Major corporate support for the Nature collection was provided by Canon U.S.A. and SC Johnson. Additional support was provided by the Corporation for Public Broadcasting and the nation’s public television stations.
The traditional view of animal behavior is that it is driven by inherited, innate instincts, but recent scientific research is revealing a larger role for complex cognitive processes among many species. The lesson will explore some of the more commonly accepted indicators of animal intelligence as demonstrated by the most brainy of all birds—the raven.
Students will first explore a series of science Web sites to compile a list of certain animal behaviors and abilities that indicate higher intelligence. They will then find and analyze examples of these behaviors and abilities as demonstrated by ravens in selected segments from the Nature episode “Ravens.” Based on what they learn, students will then work in groups to create a theoretical intelligence-challenging “obstacle course” for ravens.
This lesson could be used following (or in conjunction with) the lesson “Symbiotic Strategies.”
Students will be able to:
Three 45-minute class periods
For each student:
A site from PBS’s NOVA exploring the most current understanding of bird brain physiology, revealing a less instinctive and more cognitive brain structure than has traditionally been thought.
A site from the Smithsonian National Zoological Park addressing some of the basic difficulties in determining bird intelligence.
A Nature site from PBS describing the intelligent behavior of four different species.
A site from North Carolina State University featuring descriptions of the different types of symbiotic relationships among animals.
A PBS site exploring intelligent behavior in various bird species.
Prior to teaching this lesson, you will need to:
Preview all of the video segments and Web sites used in the lesson.
Download the video segments used in the lesson to your classroom computer, or prepare to watch them using your classroom's Internet connection.
Bookmark the Web sites used in the lesson on each computer in your classroom. Using a social bookmarking tools such as del.icio.us or diigo (or an online bookmarking utility such as portaportal) will allow you to organize all the links in a central location.
Gather the necessary materials listed above in advance of teaching the lesson. Download and print the “Raven Reason” student organizer and make copies for each student in your classroom.
Note that the computer requirements in the “Materials” section reflect an ideal arrangement. You may find it necessary to divide the class into a number of groups equal to the computers available, adjusting the lesson instructions accordingly.
Explain that up until very recently, the accepted view was that birds were almost robotic, with even their most complex behaviors governed by a genetically pre-programmed set of instincts. Recent scientific research, however, is revealing quite a different reality. Have students go to the Bird Brain Web site and scroll to the diagrams at the bottom of the page. Provide a focus for media interaction by asking how the “classic view” of the songbird brain compares with the human brain? (The bird brain is overwhelmingly instinctive compared to the more cognitive—that is, more thoughtful—human brain.) What do the diagrams tell us about how the “modern” view of bird brains has changed from the “classic” view? (Bird brains are now understood to be much more cognitive and less instinctive than once believed.)
Explain that determining an animal’s cognitive intelligence can be very difficult. Write the following question on a blackboard or whiteboard: “What basic abilities or behaviors might indicate cognitive intelligence in a species?” Have students go to the Measuring Intelligence Web site. Allow them 5 minutes to read the page, providing them with a focus for media interaction by asking how this article answers the question on the board (i.e. how it defines a “thinking” animal). (“Thinking animals are those that can demonstrate flexibility when faced with new environments and challenges.”) Ask how this quality might be described in one word. (Adaptability.) Write this answer on the board beneath the question.
Have students go to the The Animal Mind Web site and click on the “Clever Creatures” link on right side of the page. Provide a focus for media interaction by again asking the question written on the board: “What basic abilities or behaviors might indicate cognitive intelligence in a species?” Have students click on all four species pictured. After allowing 2 minutes for students to read about the intelligent behavior of the four species listed, review the focus question. Ask students to volunteer their responses as you write them on the board. Answers should include:
Ask if anyone can think of any other general abilities or behaviors that might indicate cognitive intelligence in an animal? Accept all answers, but encourage the following:
Distribute copies of Raven Reason. Explain that it lists some of the behaviors and abilities that tend to indicate high levels of cognitive intelligence in animals. (Note that certain answers given by students in the previous section might not be specifically mentioned here.) Tell students that the rest of this lesson will be spent examining video segments featuring the most intelligent of all birds—the raven—and that as their various abilities and behaviors are discussed, students should make note of them in the appropriate row of the organizer.
Tell students they will first be looking at a video segment describing ravens’ primary intelligence attribute: adaptability. Provide students with a focus for media interaction by asking why ravens need to be intelligent. Play the Raven Adaptability QuickTime Video. Pause the segment after the narrator says “…but they prefer meat.” Review the focus question: Why do ravens need to be intelligent? (To adapt to different environments and find food.) Ask students what ravens eat. (Preferably meat, but almost anything that’s available.) What type of animal does this makes them? (An omnivore.) Explain that ravens don’t kill their own food, relying instead on finding animals that are already dead. Ask students what type of animals this makes them? (Scavengers.)
Tell students they will now be watching the rest of the segment. Provide them with a focus for media interaction by asking how coyotes help ravens get food. Play the segment through to the end. Review the focus question: How do coyotes help ravens get food? (They rip open the tough hides of dead animals so ravens can eat the insides.)
Explain that the manner in which ravens benefit from coyotes is an example of a symbiosis, which describes a close ecological relationship between different species. Ask students if they know which type of symbiosis this is. (Accept all answers.) Have students go to the Symbiosis Web site and provide them with a focus for media interaction by asking which of the five types of symbiotic relationship exists between the raven and the coyote, based upon what they’ve just seen in the segment. (Commensalism, in which one species benefits and the other is unaffected. In this case, the Raven benefits from the coyote’s teeth and jaws without doing the coyote any harm. Note that some students may answer “parasitism” because the ravens have taken some of the coyotes’ food; explain that this doesn’t actually do the coyotes any biological “harm.”)
Tell students that ravens have been observed to act as airborne guides for coyotes, using their keen eyesight to lead their earthbound partners to carrion (dead animals), which the ravens need their help to tear open. Based on this new information, ask students how they might classify the symbiotic relationship between ravens and coyotes differently according to the five categories listed on the “Symbiosis” Web site? (Mutualism, in which both species benefit.) Explain that ravens’ ability to exploit and cooperate with other animals in symbiotic relationships is a sign of their high intelligence.
Remind students that adaptability is another hallmark of intelligence, and tell them that they will now be watching a segment in which ravens demonstrate this attribute by establishing a different symbiotic relationship to survive in harsh winter environments. Provide a focus for media interaction by asking what other intelligence attribute is being displayed by the raven in the segment. Play Segment 2, Feeding Time QuickTime Video. Pause the segment after the narrator says “This raven knows it can only carry four hot dogs, so it counts them out.” Review the focus question: What other higher intelligence attribute is being displayed by the raven? (Counting.) What might have drawn ravens to garbage dumpsters in the winter? (They’re always in the same place and are full of food even in winter, when food is hard to find in the wilderness.) Ask students what type of symbiotic relationship exists between humans and ravens. (Commensalism. The ravens benefit from humans’ activity without harming them—aside from maybe making a mess.)
Tell students that they will now be continuing with the segment, providing them with a focus for media interaction by asking what other intelligence attributes these ravens demonstrate as they gather to feed. Resume playing segment 2, pausing after the narrator says “Ravens have learned to use other creatures' skills to their own advantage.” Review the focus question: What other intelligence attributes are these ravens demonstrating as they gather to feed? (Communication—by both squawks and body language—and social interaction.) Ask what types of body language the ravens use to communicate aggressiveness or formidability? (They puff their head, throat, and leg feathers.) Why might they adopt this posture? (To indicate their territorialism or hierarchical status.)
Remind students about the raven with the four hot dogs. Ask students if they think it will be able to eat all four of those hot dogs at once? (Probably not.) What do they think it will do with the extras? (Accept all answers.) Tell students they will now be watching a segment that will reveal the raven’s strategy for keeping extra food. Provide a focus for media interaction by asking what this strategy is called. Resume playing Segment 2 through to the end. Review the focus question: What is the raven’s strategy for keeping extra food called? (Caching.) What intelligence attribute is required for this task? (Memory—to recall where food was hidden.)
Explain that while the raven’s act of caching-to say nothing of cache theft by other ravens-might seem to indicate "an every bird for itself" mentality within the species, as students will see in this next segment, there is actually ample evidence that within their own communities, young ravens in particular can be quite cooperative. Provide a focus for media interaction by asking students why a young raven, having found food, might call other young ravens to help eat it. Play Segment 3, The Roost QuickTime Video. Review the focus question, asking why a young raven, having found food, might call other young ravens to help it eat. (The younger ravens find safety in numbers from older ravens that might otherwise take the food for themselves.) Explain that this ability to share and cooperate is another example of ravens’ very practical intelligence, allowing them to survive as a collective where they might fail as individuals.
Explain that, as with most birds (and animals more generally), basic survival instincts like finding food have led ravens to develop quite intelligent behavior, but that some of their most impressive demonstrations of intelligence are found through experiments that test their cognitive capacity to solve more complex or abstract problems. Tell students that the last segment they will be watching will show a few of these experiments, conducted by zoologist and raven expert Berndt Heinrich. Explain that to guarantee the integrity of his results, Heinrich needed to ensure that his ravens had no prior experience with the challenges of his tests. Ask students how they think he might have accomplished this? (Accept all answers.) Provide a focus for media interaction by asking students how Heinrich ensures that his ravens have no prior experience with the test. Play Segment 4, Testing Intelligence QuickTime Video. Review the focus question: How did Heinrich ensure that his ravens have no prior experience with the tests? (He raised them himself). Ask students what about the ravens’ performance in the tests they find impressive. (Accept all answers, but encourage an appreciation that without any prior training or condition, they solved relatively complex spatial relation problems using coordinated movements of their feet and beak.) Ask: Do all birds pass this test? (No—even the raven’s close cousin the crow fails.) Explain that success in this test indicates that ravens possess not only complex instinctive knowledge, but also cognitive comprehension of more abstract spatial relations.
Explain that even crows—demonstrably less intelligent than their raven cousins—exhibit types of intelligent behavior once thought exclusive to mammals or even just humans. Have students log onto the Nutcrackers Web site and have them read the first five paragraphs, providing a focus for media interaction by asking why crows can’t crack open walnuts with the same technique they use for clams? (Because the walnut’s soft outer shell doesn’t break on rocks.) Ask students how they think crows might have figured out this nutcracking strategy. (Accept all answers.) Explain that the crow’s complex nutcracking technique is probably based upon their synthesized comprehension of several simpler observations. Ask the class what these might be. Answers may vary, but should include:
Ask students if they think the crow’s nutcracking technique is an example of tool use. (Accept all answers. Explain that the answer depends on how strictly one defines a tool; despite the obvious work that the car’s wheels are doing for the crow, the nutcracking crows aren’t actually manipulating the car’s wheels.) Tell students that there are other types of crows that do use tools by even the strictest definition. Have them scroll down the “Nutcrackers” Web page until they get to the paragraph adjacent to the second photo, which starts “On the Pacific island of New Caledonia...” Tell students to read that and the following three paragraphs, providing a focus for media interaction by asking whether scientists know for sure whether tool making and use among crows is genetically inherited or cognitively learned. (They do not.)
12. Ask students if they think the walnut-cracking technique of the Japanese crows is genetically inherited or cognitively learned? (Accept all answers, but encourage an understanding that the behavior has only been observed for the past 20 years or so in Japan—less in California—and ;cars themselves have only existed for about 100 years—too short a time for such complex behavior to have evolved genetically.)
History
Ravens have traditionally been perceived as birds of ill-omen. Have students investigate why this is, and then have them explore more positive perceptions of ravens in three different cultural contexts: the Tower of London, traditional Native American folklore of the Pacific Northwest, and Norse mythology.
English/Language Arts
Have students read and critically analyze Edgar Allan Poe’s poem “The Raven,” asking them to consider which cultural perceptions of ravens it draws upon, and has itself perpetuated. Students can also go to an excellent interactive Web site about "The Raven."
Divide the class into groups of four. Explain that each group will be designing a single theoretical intelligence-testing challenge (similar to those shown in the “Testing Intelligence” segment) for ravens based upon this bird’s demonstrated intelligence attributes (i.e. the content of the “Raven Reason” student organizer). Each group should make drawings or diagrams to help explain and describe its challenge. Explain that each group’s challenge will be “graded” by the other groups on a point scale according to the following criteria:
Allow at least a half hour for groups to complete this activity, carrying over into the next day’s class if necessary (with students brainstorming ideas as homework in between). Then have each group designate a speaker to present its challenge to the class. After each presentation, allow at least five minutes for the other groups to confer and determine the score they will award it. The scores assigned by each group should be kept secret until all groups have presented, at which time the scores will be revealed and tallied. Explain that these kinds of tests lead us new ideas that expand our understanding of behavior in living things.
Invite a local ornithologist, zoologist, or birdkeeper to bring a raven to class for a “show and tell.” Alternatively, visit a local zoo or aviary to see ravens in a more natural habitat.